7 国際: 2017年9月アーカイブ
Authors: Takano, Y., Ukezono, M., Nakashima, S. F. (中嶋智史), Takahashi, N., & Hironaka, N.
Title: Learning of efficient behaviour in spatial exploration through observation of behaviour of conspecific in laboratory rats.
Journal(書誌情報): Royal Society Open Science, 4, 170121
doi: DOI: 10.1098/rsos.170121
論文URL: http://rsos.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/4/9/170121
Abstract: Recent studies have suggested that rodent behaviour is influenced by the behaviour of surrounding conspecifics (e.g. emotional contagion and prosocial behaviour). However, little is known about deferred imitation and complex observational learning in rats. The purpose of this study was to reveal whether rats can learn from another rat's experiences. In a maze, observer rats watched the foraging behaviour of other rats (demonstrators) and then foraged in turn. The results showed that demonstrators explored inefficiently, but observers explored more efficiently after observing inefficient exploration by the demonstrators. This observational learning probably involved the acquisition of an efficient strategy through spatial exploration.
著者Contact先の email: ytakano[at]mail.doshisha.ac.jp([at]を@に変更してください。)
Authors: Masataka Nakayama(中山真孝)and Satoru Saito(齊藤智)
Title: Position-element frequency learning is dissociable from Hebb repetition learning
Journal(書誌情報): Journal of Memory and Language, Volume 94, June 2017, Pages 235-253
doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.11.007
論文URL: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0749596X16302479?via%3Dihub
Abstract:
Position-element association has been extensively investigated as a mechanism for serial order memory and has been widely implemented in models of short-term memory. This study examined whether and how the position-element association is learned as a form of long-term knowledge dissociated from other forms of knowledge such as sequence knowledge acquired in the Hebb list repetition paradigm. Laboratory learning experiments demonstrated that repeated exposure to a specific position-element association facilitated subsequent recall of the position-element association with experimental control of other aspects of to-be-learned statistical structure of the artificial phonotactics. The experiments also demonstrated that the positional frequency learning was more gradual than Hebb list learning, suggesting a dissociation of the two forms of learning. Functional roles of these two forms of learning were discussed.
著者Contact先の email:
masataka.nakayama.mn [at] gmail.com (M. Nakayama), or saito.satoru.2z [at] kyoto-u.ac.jp (S. Saito).
Title:Neurodevelopmental hypothesis about the etiology of autism spectrum disorders.
Journal(書誌情報): Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00354
論文URL:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=Neurodevelopmental+
hypothesis+about+the+etiology+of+autism+spectrum+disorders.
Abstract:
Previous models or hypotheses of autism spectral disorder (ASD) failed to take into full consideration the chronological and causal developmental trajectory, leading to the emergence of diverse phenotypes through a complex interaction between individual etiologies and environmental factors. Those phenotypes include persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction (criteria A in DSM-5), and restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities (criteria B in DSM-5). In this article, we proposed a domain-general model that can explain criteria in DSM-5 based on the assumption that the same etiological mechanism would trigger the various phenotypes observed in different individuals with ASD. In the model, we assumed the following joint causes as the etiology of autism: (1) Hypoplasia of the pons in the brainstem, occurring immediately following neural tube closure; and (2) Deficiency in the GABA (γ-aminobutyric acid) developmental switch during the perinatal period. Microstructural abnormalities of the pons directly affect both the structural and functional development of the brain areas strongly connected to it, especially amygdala. The impairment of GABA switch could not only lead to the deterioration of inhibitory processing in the neural network, but could also cause abnormal cytoarchitecture. We introduced a perspective that atypical development in both brain structure and function can give full explanation of diverse phenotypes and pathogenetic mechanism of ASD. Finally, we discussed about neural mechanisms underlying the phenotypic characteristics of ASD that are not described in DSM-5 but should be considered as important foundation: sleep, global precedence, categorical perception, intelligence, interoception and motor control.
著者Contact先の email:meitz100[at]fork.ocn.ne.jp([at]を@に変更してください。)
Authors: Toshiki Saito, Rui Nouchi, Hikari Kinjo, Ryuta Kawashima
Title:Gaze Bias in Preference Judgments by Younger and Older Adults
Journal(書誌情報): Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
doi:10.3389/fnagi.2017.00285
論文URL:http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00285
Abstract:Individuals' gaze behavior reflects the choice they will ultimately make. For example, people confronting a choice among multiple stimuli tend to look longer at stimuli that are subsequently chosen than at other stimuli. This tendency, called the gaze bias effect, is a key aspect of visual decision-making. Nevertheless, no study has examined the generality of the gaze bias effect in older adults. Here, we used a two-alternative forced-choice task (2AFC) to compare the gaze behavior reflective of different stages of decision processes demonstrated by younger and older adults. Participants who had viewed two faces were instructed to choose the one that they liked/disliked or the one that they judged to be more/less similar to their own face. Their eye movements were tracked while they chose. The results show that the gaze bias effect occurred during the remaining time in both age groups irrespective of the decision type. However, no gaze bias effect was observed for the preference judgment during the first dwell time. Our study demonstrated that the gaze bias during the remaining time occurred regardless of decision-making task and age. Further study using diverse participants, such as clinic patients or infants, may help to generalize the gaze bias effect and to elucidate the mechanisms underlying the gaze bias.
著者Contact先の email: toshiki.saito[at]med.tohoku.ac.jp([at]を@に変更してください。)
② Authors: Kobayashi, Y., Matsushita, S., & Morikawa, K. (小林勇輝・松下戦具・森川和則) Title: Effects of lip color on perceived lightness of human facial skin (唇の色が肌の明るさ知覚に与える効果) Journal(書誌情報): i-Perception, 8(4), 2017 doi: 10.1177/2041669517717500 論文URL: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/2041669517717500
Abstract: Whereas geometric illusions in human faces have been reported by several studies, illusions of color or lightness in faces have seldom been explored. Here, we psychophysically investigated whether lip color influences facial skin's perceived lightness. Results of Experiment 1 demonstrated that redder lips lightened and darker lips darkened the perceived complexion. These lightness or darkness inducing effects differ from the classical illusion of lightness contrast in nonface objects for two reasons. First, illusory effects are more assimilative than contrastive. Second, the inducing area (i.e., lips) is much smaller than the influenced area (facial skin). Experiment 2 showed that the assimilative lightness induction was caused by holistic processing of faces. This is the first study to scientifically substantiate the claim of cosmetics manufacturers and makeup artists that lip colors can alter perceived facial skin color. Implications for face perception, lightness illusion, and perceptual effects of cosmetics are discussed. 著者Contact先の email: y-kobayashi[at]hus.osaka-u.ac.jp (小林勇輝) 日本語によるコメント(オプション,200-300字で): 化粧業界でときたま取り上げられる「口紅を使うことで肌の明るさが変わって見える」という言説を,心理物理学的手法によって検証しました。結果として,唇を暗くすることで肌が暗く見え,唇を赤くすることで肌が明るく見えることが明らかとなりました。この現象は①同化傾向の色誘導である点②小領域が大領域に影響する点の2点において一般物体の色誘導と異なる傾向を示しています。倒立顔を用いた実験ではこの効果が著しく弱まることが確認され,全体処理によって引き起こされる顔特有の色誘導現象があることが示されました。(小林)
① Authors: Muto, H., Matsushita, S., & Morikawa, K. (武藤拓之・松下戦具・森川和則) Title: Spatial perspective taking mediated by whole-body motor simulation (空間的視点取得は全身の運動シミュレーションに媒介される) Journal(書誌情報): Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Advance online publication, 2017. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000464 論文URL: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28795837 Abstract: Humans can envision the world from other people's viewpoints. To explore the embodied process of such spatial perspective taking, we examined whether action related to a whole-body movement modulates performance on spatial perspective-taking tasks. Results showed that when participants responded by putting their left/right foot or left/right hand forward, actions congruent with a movement's direction (clockwise/counterclockwise) reduced RTs relative to incongruent actions. In contrast, actions irrelevant to a movement (a left/right hand index-finger response) did not affect performance. Furthermore, we demonstrated that this response congruency effect cannot be explained by either spatial stimulus-response compatibility or sensorimotor interference. These results support the involvement of simulated whole-body movement in spatial perspective taking. Moreover, the findings revealed faster foot responses than hand responses during spatial perspective taking, whereas the opposite result was obtained during a simple orientation judgment task without spatial perspective taking. Overall, our findings highlight the important role of motor simulation in spatial perspective taking. 著者Contact先の email: h_muto[at]hus.osaka-u.ac.jp (武藤拓之) 日本語によるコメント(オプション,200-300字で): 自分とは異なる視点から見た物の見え方を想像する認知過程(空間的視点取得)に関する研究です。本研究は,空間的視点取得の際に,取得する視点の位置まで自分の身体を回転させる運動シミュレーションが行われていることを5つの行動実験で実証しました。具体的には,(1) 取得する視点までの移動方向と一致した手足の動作を反応に用いた時に,不一致の動作を用いた時よりも素早く視点取得ができること(反応一致性効果),および (2) 空間的視点取得課題において,手よりも足の反応が速くなること(足の優位性)を報告しています。これらの結果は,空間的視点取得の認知過程が進化的および発達的に身体性に根差している可能性を示唆しています。(武藤)